(Press-News.org) It’s a question that has puzzled thinkers for centuries: Are we humans alone in our pursuit of fairness and the frustration we feel when others get what we want?
In recent years, evolutionary psychologists have suggested that we’re not all that special. Animals, from corvids to capuchin monkeys, express what humans might recognize as jealousy when, for example, they are passed over for a sought-after snack. Many argue this is evidence we are not alone in our aversion toward unfairness.
But new research from the University of California, Berkeley, makes the case that humans might be unique after all.
Using data from 23 studies of what psychologists call “inequity aversion,” Berkeley researchers combed through results of more than 60,000 observations involving 18 animal species. In what they said was the “largest empirical investigation of non-human inequity aversion to date,” the team reconstructed data analyses and used a new metric that adds depth to the concept of fairness.
“We can’t make the claim that animals experience jealousy based on this data,” said Oded Ritov, a fourth-year Ph.D. candidate in UC Berkeley’s Department of Psychology. “If there is an effect, it’s very weak and might show up in very specific settings.
“But it’s nothing like what we see in humans in terms of our deep-seated sense of fairness.”
The meta-analysis was published Nov. 27 in the journal Proceedings of the Royal Society B.
Ritov, the paper’s first author, studies how human behaviors have evolved, how much of it is baked into our big brains and what is learned through our complicated cultures. He also researches non-human animals to better understand what makes humans special.
Our sense of fairness in distributing resources may be a key reason we were able to build shelters, share food and develop more complex societies. To be sure, humans have differing perceptions of fairness. But core to the concept is what psychologists call “inequity aversion,” a disinclination for the unequal distribution of resources and judgments on how things should be shared.
Examples are all around us, as siblings or parents of young children can attest. When one child is given something nicer than the other, outbursts often follow. It’s not just that the youngster didn’t get a toy or treat; someone else did instead. This reaction shows aversion toward inequity.
It’s long been debated just how different that perception of fairness is for non-human animals.
Primatologist Frans de Waal’s landmark study and accompanying viral video of an endearing capuchin monkey lashing out made the case that such animals show an understanding of inequity aversion that’s remarkably similar to that of human children. All was well and good when both monkeys were given a slice of cucumber. But when researchers gave one a grape, the cucumber-munching chimp seemed to grow jealous, flung the cucumber back at the researcher and rattled the cage wall in protest.
De Waal and others said this and subsequent experiments supported the claim that humans are not alone in our sense of fairness. Similar studies on corvids, dogs and mice have likewise been reported to show inequity aversion.
Ritov said that might be the “straightforward, and perhaps anthropomorphic, interpretation.” But that doesn’t mean it’s the only one.
He said many of these studies have been hampered by a replication crisis that has long plagued psychology and other disciplines. Findings might be compelling, but they’re based on small sample sizes and are difficult to repeat, clouding their broader contributions to science.
“We thought it’d be a valuable contribution to try to pull together as much data as we can on this question and see what kind of pattern emerges with the larger dataset,” Ritov said.
The pattern that emerged after rerunning the data with a new variable suggests the animals weren’t displaying jealousy. They were actually disappointed after expecting a grape based on past research behavior. Follow-up studies elicited similar outrage in monkeys, even when the grapes were placed in an empty cage — where there was no other monkey to be jealous of.
“We think that the rejections are a form of social protest,” Ritov said. “But what animals are protesting isn’t receiving less than someone else. Rather, it seems like they’re protesting the human not treating them as well as they could.”
Perhaps the reaction was never about inequity aversion, Ritov argues. It was about unmet expectations. And that’s something that humans and non-human animals alike can relate to.
END
Do animals get jealous like people? Researchers say it’s complicated.
New UC Berkeley research looks at years of studies into whether non-human animals have a similar sense of fairness. The answer is more nuanced than it may seem.
2024-12-12
ELSE PRESS RELEASES FROM THIS DATE:
Social risks impede cancer screening, even with access to care
2024-12-12
Researchers at University of California San Diego and collaborating institutions have shed light on the ways that social risks, such as housing or food insecurity, pose barriers to routine cancer screenings. The study, published in JNCI Cancer Spectrum, found that patients experiencing social risks were less likely to receive orders for cancer screenings and even less likely to complete screenings when ordered. The study also found that patients experiencing social risks had higher rates of primary care ...
Examining gender inequality in academic publishing
2024-12-12
Editors of academic journals hold an influential position in their field. They have decision-making power over which authors and papers get published, set journal policy, and help shape the trajectory of their discipline. It is also a role in which women are frequently underrepresented.
Assistant professor of accounting Sebastian Tideman-Frappart and several colleagues set out to fill a knowledge gap about this issue in the field of management science by tracking gender diversity in world-leading management journals over time. The resulting article – co-authored by Brooke Gazdag, associate professor of management at Kühne Logistics University; Jamie Gloor, assistant professor ...
UH researchers characterize keys to successful pregnancy in humpback whales
2024-12-12
In a breakthrough study published this week in The Journal of Physiology, researchers at the Marine Mammal Research Program (MMRP) at the University of Hawaiʻi at Manoa's Hawaiʻi Institute of Marine Biology (HIMB) demystify the energetic cost of humpback pregnancy and shed light on the unique vulnerabilities of migratory humpback mothers-to-be. With an arsenal of tools that range from cutting-edge technology to historical whaling records, the research was done in close partnership with Alaska ...
Policy Forum: Considering risks of “mirror life” before it is created
2024-12-12
In a Policy Forum, scientists discuss lifeforms composed of mirror-image biological molecules – also known as “mirror life” – and say creation of such lifeforms, which could evade immune mechanisms and predators, warrants careful consideration. The hallmark of mirror organisms is reversed chirality – a feature that would render them resistant to normal forms of biological degradation, making them useful for applications like long-lasting therapies. While these organisms haven’t yet been observed in nature, and the capability to create them is likely at least ...
Breakthrough of the Year: A drug that prevents HIV infection, providing six months of protection per shot
2024-12-12
As its 2024 Breakthrough of The Year, Science has named the development of lenacapavir – a promising new injectable drug that prevents HIV infection. The award also recognizes related work surrounding gaining a new understanding of the structure and function of HIV’s capsid protein. Despite decades of advancements, HIV continues to infect more than a million people annually, with a vaccine remaining elusive. However, a new injectable drug, lenacapavir, offers hope by providing six ...
Heatwave led to catastrophic and persistent loss of Alaska’s dominant seabird
2024-12-12
The 2014-2016 Pacific marine heatwave wiped out more than half – roughly 4 million – of Alaska’s common murre (Uria aalge) seabirds, representing the largest documented vertebrate die-off linked to warming oceans, according to a new study. “Although research on the impacts of global warming on marine birds has clearly suggested major shifts in species’ ranges and abundance, documented changes have been gradual (years to decades),” write the authors. “To our knowledge, this study is the first to show that climate impacts can be swift (1 ...
Genomic analysis refines timing of Neandertal admixture – and its impact on modern humans
2024-12-12
A genomic study encompassing more than 300 genomes spanning the last 50,000 years has revealed how a single wave of Neandertal gene flow into early modern humans left an indelible mark on human evolution. Among other findings, the study reports that modern humans acquired several Neanderthal genes that ended up being advantageous to our lineage, including those involved in skin pigmentation, immune response, and metabolism. To date, sequencing of Neanderthal and Denisovan genomes has revealed substantial gene flow between these archaic hominins ...
Superflares once per century
2024-12-12
There is no question that the Sun is a temperamental star, as alone this year’s unusually strong solar storms prove. Some of them led to remarkable auroras even at low latitudes. But can our star become even more furious? Evidence of the most violent solar “tantrums” can be found in prehistoric tree trunks and in samples of millennia-old glacial ice. However, from these indirect sources, the frequency of superflares cannot be determined. And direct measurements of the amount of radiation reaching the Earth from the Sun have only been available since the ...
A new timeline for Neanderthal interbreeding with modern humans
2024-12-12
A new analysis of DNA from ancient modern humans (Homo sapiens) in Europe and Asia has determined, more precisely than ever, the time period during which Neanderthals interbred with modern humans, starting about 50,500 years ago and lasting about 7,000 years — until Neanderthals began to disappear.
That interbreeding left Eurasians with many genes inherited from our Neanderthal ancestors, which in total make up between 1% and 2% of our genomes today.
The genome-based estimate is consistent with archeological evidence that modern humans and Neanderthals lived side-by-side in Eurasia for between 6,000 and 7,000 years. The analysis, which involved present-day human genomes ...
New timeline for Neandertal gene flow event
2024-12-12
The study of ancient DNA has greatly advanced our knowledge of human evolution, including the discovery of gene flow from Neandertals into the common ancestors of modern humans. Neandertals and modern humans diverged about 500,000 years ago, with Neandertals living in Eurasia for the past 300,000 years. Then, sometime around 40,000 to 60,000 years ago, modern human groups left Africa and spread across Eurasia, encountering Neandertals along the way. As a result, most non-Africans harbor one to two percent ...
LAST 30 PRESS RELEASES:
New study reveals how reduced rainfall threatens plant diversity
New study reveals optimized in vitro fertilization techniques to boost coral restoration efforts in the Caribbean
No evidence that maternal sickness during pregnancy causes autism
Healthy gut bacteria that feed on sugar analyzed for the first time
240-year-old drug could save UK National Health Service £100 million a year treating common heart rhythm disorder
Detections of poliovirus in sewage samples require enhanced routine and catch-up vaccination and increased surveillance, according to ECDC report
Scientists unlock ice-repelling secrets of polar bear fur for sustainable anti-freezing solutions
Ear muscle we thought humans didn’t use — except for wiggling our ears — actually activates when people listen hard
COVID-19 pandemic drove significant rise in patients choosing to leave ERs before medically recommended
Burn grasslands to maintain them: What is good for biodiversity?
Ventilation in hospitals could cause viruses to spread further
New study finds high concentrations of plastics in the placentae of infants born prematurely
New robotic surgical systems revolutionizing patient care
New MSK research a step toward off-the-shelf CAR T cell therapy for cancer
UTEP professor wins prestigious research award from American Psychological Association
New national study finds homicide and suicide is the #1 cause of maternal death in the U.S.
Women’s pelvic tissue tears during childbirth unstudied, until now
Earth scientists study Sikkim flood in India to help others prepare for similar disasters
Leveraging data to improve health equity and care
Why you shouldn’t scratch an itchy rash: New study explains
Linking citation and retraction data aids in responsible research evaluation
Antibody treatment prevents severe bird flu in monkeys
Polar bear energetic model reveals drivers of polar bear population decline
Socioeconomic and political stability bolstered wild tiger recovery in India
Scratching an itch promotes antibacterial inflammation
Drivers, causes and impacts of the 2023 Sikkim flood in India
Most engineered human cells created for studying disease
Polar bear population decline the direct result of extended ‘energy deficit’ due to lack of food
Lifecycle Journal launches: A new vision for scholarly publishing
Ancient DNA analyses bring to life the 11,000-year intertwined genomic history of sheep and humans
[Press-News.org] Do animals get jealous like people? Researchers say it’s complicated.New UC Berkeley research looks at years of studies into whether non-human animals have a similar sense of fairness. The answer is more nuanced than it may seem.