(Press-News.org) Contact information: Tom Abate
tabate@stanford.edu
650-736-2245
Stanford School of Engineering
Stanford researchers surprised to find how neural circuits identify information needed for decisions
Multitasking neurons filter and decide, confounding the conventional wisdom
While eating lunch you notice an insect buzzing around your plate. Its color and motion could both influence how you respond. If the insect was yellow and black you might decide it was a bee and move away. Conversely, you might simply be annoyed at the buzzing motion and shoo the insect away. You perceive both color and motion, and decide based on the circumstances. Our brains make such contextual decisions in a heartbeat. The mystery is how.
In an article published November 7th in the journal Nature, a team of Stanford neuroscientists and engineers delve into this decision-making process and report some findings that confound the conventional wisdom.
Until now, neuroscientists have believed that decisions of this sort involved two steps: one group of neurons that performed a gating function to ascertain whether motion or color was most relevant to the situation, and a second group of neurons that considered only the sensory input relevant to making a decision under the circumstances.
But in a study that combined brain recordings from trained monkeys, and a sophisticated computer model based on that biological data, Stanford neuroscientist William Newsome and three coauthors discovered that the entire decision-making process may occur in a localized region of the prefrontal cortex.
In this region of the brain, located in the frontal lobes just behind the forehead, they found that color and motion signals converged in a specific circuit of neurons. Based on their experimental evidence and computer simulations, the scientists hypothesized that these neurons act together to make two snap judgments: is color or motion the most relevant sensory input in the current context, and what action to take as a result.
"We were quite surprised," said Newsome, the Harman Family Provostial Professor at the Stanford School of Medicine and lead author.
He and first author Valerio Mante, a former Stanford neurobiologist now at the University of Zurich and the Swiss Federal Institute of Technology, had begun the experiment expecting to find that the irrelevant signal, whether color or motion, would be gated out of the circuit long before the decision-making neurons went into action.
"What we saw instead was this complicated mix of signals that we could measure, but whose meaning and underlying mechanism we couldn't understand," Newsome said. "These signals held information about the color and motion of the stimulus, which stimulus dimension was most relevant, and the decision that the monkeys made. But the signals were profoundly mixed up at the single neuron level. We decided there was a lot more we needed to learn about these neurons, and that the key to unlocking the secret might lie in a population level analysis of the circuit activity."
To solve this brain puzzle the neurobiologists began a cross-disciplinary collaboration with Krishna Shenoy, a professor of electrical engineering at Stanford, and David Sussillo, co-first author on the paper and a postdoctoral scholar in Shenoy's lab.
Sussillo created a software model to simulate how these neurons worked. The idea was to build a model sophisticated enough to mimic the decision-making process, but easier to study than taking repeated electrical readings from a brain.
The general model architecture they used is called a recurrent neural network: a set of software modules designed to accept inputs and perform tasks similar to how biological neurons operate. The scientists designed this artificial neural network using computational techniques that enabled the software model to make itself more proficient at decision-making over time.
"We challenged the artificial system to solve a problem analogous to the one given to the monkeys," Sussillo explained. "But we didn't tell the neural network how to solve the problem."
As a result, once the artificial network learned to solve the task, the scientists could study the model to develop inferences about how the biological neurons might be working.
The entire process was grounded in the biological experiments.
The neuroscientists trained two macaque monkeys to view a random-dot visual display that had two different features—motion and color. For any given presentation, the dots could move to the right or left, and the color could be red or green. The monkeys were taught to use sideways glances to answer two different questions depending on the currently instructed "rule" or context. Were there more red or green dots (ignore the motion)? Or, were the dots moving to the left or right (ignore the color)?
Eye-tracking instruments recorded the glances, or saccades, that the monkeys used to register their responses. Their answers were correlated with recordings of neuronal activity taken directly from an area in the prefrontal cortex known to control saccadic eye movements.
The neuroscientists collected 1,402 such experimental measurements. Each time the monkeys were asked one or the other question. The idea was to obtain brain recordings at the moment when the monkeys saw a visual cue that established the context (either the red/green or left/right question), and what decision the animal made regarding color or direction of motion.
It was the puzzling mish-mash of signals in the brain recordings from these experiments that prompted the scientists to build the recurrent neural network as a way to rerun the experiment, in a simulated way, time and time again.
As the four researchers became confident that their software simulations accurately mirrored the actual biological behavior, they studied the model to learn exactly how it solved the task. This allowed them to form a hypothesis about what was occurring in that patch of neurons in the prefrontal cortex where perception and decision occurred.
"The idea is really very simple," Sussillo explained.
Their hypothesis revolves around two mathematical concepts: a line attractor and a selection vector.
The entire group of neurons being studied received sensory data about both the color and the motion of the dots.
The line attractor is a mathematical representation for the amount of information that this group of neurons was getting about either of the relevant inputs, color or motion.
The selection vector represented how the model responded when the experimenters flashed one of the two questions: red or green, left or right?
What the model showed was that when the question pertained to color, the selection vector directed the artificial neurons to accept color information while ignoring the irrelevant motion information. Color data became the line attractor. After a split second these neurons registered a decision, choosing the red or green answer based on the data they were supplied.
If question was about motion, the selection vector directed motion information to the line attractor and the artificial neurons chose left or right.
"The amazing part is that a single neuronal circuit is doing all of this," Sussillo says. "If our model is correct, then almost all neurons in this biological circuit appear to be contributing to almost all parts of the information selection and decision-making mechanism."
Newsome put it like this: "We think that all of these neurons are interested in everything that's going on, but they're interested to different degrees. They're multitasking like crazy."
Other researchers who are aware of the work but were not directly involved are commenting on the paper.
"This is a spectacular example of excellent experimentation combined with clever data analysis and creative theoretical modeling," said Larry Abbott, Co-Director of the Center for Theoretical Neuroscience and the William Bloor Professor, Neuroscience, Physiology & Cellular Biophysics, Biological Sciences at Columbia University.
Christopher Harvey, a professor of neurobiology at Harvard Medical School, said the paper "provides major new hypotheses about the inner-workings of the prefrontal cortex, which is a brain area that has frequently been identified as significant for higher cognitive processes but whose mechanistic functioning has remained mysterious."
The Stanford scientists are now designing a new biological experiment to ascertain whether the interplay between selection vector and line attractor, which they deduced from their software model, can be measured in actual brain signals.
"The model predicts a very specific type of neural activity under very specific circumstances," Sussillo said. "If we can stimulate the prefrontal cortex in the right way, and then measure this activity, we will have gone a long way to proving that the model mechanism is indeed what is happening in the biological circuit."
The four researchers worked together on this paper for more than 18 months at the James H. Clark Center, home of Bio-X, Stanford's ongoing effort to stimulate interdisciplinary collaborations in the field of human biology.
INFORMATION:
The work was supported by: the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, the Air Force Research Laboratory, a Pioneer Award from the National Institutes of Health, and the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency.
Stanford researchers surprised to find how neural circuits identify information needed for decisions
Multitasking neurons filter and decide, confounding the conventional wisdom
2013-11-07
ELSE PRESS RELEASES FROM THIS DATE:
Researchers and clinicians unite to answer what will it take to achieve an AIDS-free world?
2013-11-07
Researchers and clinicians unite to answer what will it take to achieve an AIDS-free world?
November 5, 2013, San Francisco, CA—Since the onset of the AIDS pandemic more than three decades ago, researchers from the lab and physicians in the clinic have been working toward one ...
Hormone levels in women using contraception affect nerve activity involved in vessel constriction
2013-11-07
Hormone levels in women using contraception affect nerve activity involved in vessel constriction
Bethesda, Md. (Nov. 6, 2013)—After menopause, women's levels of estrogen and progesterone fall. Their formerly lower risk for heart disease equals, even surpasses, men's risk. ...
'Please feed me': The power of putting a human face on social causes
2013-11-07
'Please feed me': The power of putting a human face on social causes
Companies often put a personal face on products in an attempt to reach a deeper connection with consumers. New research suggests the same idea can be applied to social ...
CWRU study finds mending ruptures in client-therapist relationship has positive benefits
2013-11-07
CWRU study finds mending ruptures in client-therapist relationship has positive benefits
In order for prolonged exposure therapy, an evidence-based psychotherapy for post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), to reach its full potential, any misperceptions ...
Mothers' relationships can influence adolescent children's relationships, MU study finds
2013-11-07
Mothers' relationships can influence adolescent children's relationships, MU study finds
COLUMBIA, Mo. – Until now, little research has been conducted on the association between parents' friendships and the emotional well-being of their adolescent ...
UC's SmartLight more than a bright idea, it's a revolution in interior lighting ready to shine
2013-11-07
UC's SmartLight more than a bright idea, it's a revolution in interior lighting ready to shine
The innovative solar technology 'would change the equation for energy,' according to UC researchers
A pair of University of Cincinnati researchers has seen the light ...
Georgia Tech warns of threats to cloud data storage, mobile devices in latest 'emerging cyber threat'
2013-11-07
Georgia Tech warns of threats to cloud data storage, mobile devices in latest 'emerging cyber threat'
As more businesses find their way into the cloud, few engage in security measures beyond those provided by the associated cloud storage firm, a new report ...
From 1 collapsing star, 2 black holes form and fuse
2013-11-07
From 1 collapsing star, 2 black holes form and fuse
Black holes—massive objects in space with gravitational forces so strong that not even light can escape them—come in a variety of sizes. On the smaller end of the scale are the stellar-mass black holes that ...
Addicts may be seeking relief from emotional lows more than euphoric highs
2013-11-07
Addicts may be seeking relief from emotional lows more than euphoric highs
Rutgers study could lead to a better understanding of human addiction -- alcohol, tobacco and food -- as well as substance abuse
Cocaine addicts may become trapped in drug binges – not because of the euphoric ...
Early childhood educators hold the key to children's communication skills
2013-11-07
Early childhood educators hold the key to children's communication skills
High-quality interactions between children and adults foster growth
Researchers at UNC's Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute have completed ...
LAST 30 PRESS RELEASES:
Walking, moving more may lower risk of cardiovascular death for women with cancer history
Intracortical neural interfaces: Advancing technologies for freely moving animals
Post-LLM era: New horizons for AI with knowledge, collaboration, and co-evolution
“Sloshing” from celestial collisions solves mystery of how galactic clusters stay hot
Children poisoned by the synthetic opioid, fentanyl, has risen in the U.S. – eight years of national data shows
USC researchers observe mice may have a form of first aid
VUMC to develop AI technology for therapeutic antibody discovery
Unlocking the hidden proteome: The role of coding circular RNA in cancer
Advancing lung cancer treatment: Understanding the differences between LUAD and LUSC
Study reveals widening heart disease disparities in the US
The role of ubiquitination in cancer stem cell regulation
New insights into LSD1: a key regulator in disease pathogenesis
Vanderbilt lung transplant establishes new record
Revolutionizing cancer treatment: targeting EZH2 for a new era of precision medicine
Metasurface technology offers a compact way to generate multiphoton entanglement
Effort seeks to increase cancer-gene testing in primary care
Acoustofluidics-based method facilitates intracellular nanoparticle delivery
Sulfur bacteria team up to break down organic substances in the seabed
Stretching spider silk makes it stronger
Earth's orbital rhythms link timing of giant eruptions and climate change
Ammonia build-up kills liver cells but can be prevented using existing drug
New technical guidelines pave the way for widespread adoption of methane-reducing feed additives in dairy and livestock
Eradivir announces Phase 2 human challenge study of EV25 in healthy adults infected with influenza
New study finds that tooth size in Otaria byronia reflects historical shifts in population abundance
nTIDE March 2025 Jobs Report: Employment rate for people with disabilities holds steady at new plateau, despite February dip
Breakthrough cardiac regeneration research offers hope for the treatment of ischemic heart failure
Fluoride in drinking water is associated with impaired childhood cognition
New composite structure boosts polypropylene’s low-temperature toughness
While most Americans strongly support civics education in schools, partisan divide on DEI policies and free speech on college campuses remains
Revolutionizing surface science: Visualization of local dielectric properties of surfaces
[Press-News.org] Stanford researchers surprised to find how neural circuits identify information needed for decisionsMultitasking neurons filter and decide, confounding the conventional wisdom