(Press-News.org) Contact information: Dr. Swidbert Ott
so120@le.ac.uk
44-011-625-23479
University of Leicester
The company you keep shapes what you learn
Research from Universities of Leicester, Sussex and Cambridge into desert locusts discovers how your environment shapes your thinking
A team of scientists has shown how the environment shapes learning and memory by training locusts like Pavlov's dog to associate different smells with reward or punishment.
Desert locusts are notorious for their devastating swarms. However, they do not always live in swarms — they switch between a lone living 'solitary phase' and a swarming 'gregarious' phase. The two phases differ profoundly in looks, behaviour and in their life style.
The new research from the Universities of Leicester, Sussex and Cambridge examines how locusts associate odours with nutritious or toxic food. The research, published in the journal Current Biology, was supported by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT), Champalimaud Foundation, The Royal Society, the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC) and the Leverhulme Trust.
Solitary locusts rely on camouflage to evade predators, and they avoid eating toxic plants; but gregarious locusts eat these plants to 'impregnate' themselves with toxins to deter predators. The transformation to gregarious behaviour, which happens when solitary locusts are forced together into a crowd, is complete within a few hours.
Dr Swidbert Ott from the University of Leicester said: "Locusts should consider toxic food 'bad' while they live alone but 'good' when they are in a swarm, which made us wonder, how do swarming locusts learn that 'bad is the new good'?"
Such research is important because it provides new insights into how animals can quickly switch between very different life styles that are adapted to different environments.
"Our research shows how animals that undergo a profound transformation in their life style also adapt their learning and memory capabilities to cope with the new environment in which they find themselves," says Dr Jeremy Niven from the University of Sussex.
Dr Patrício Simões, who carried out the experiments as part of his doctoral study at the University of Cambridge and the Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, explains: "When we presented solitary locusts with an unfamiliar odour together with toxic food, they assigned it an aversive ('bad') value. But if the locust is in a crowd and starting to change towards gregarious, it assigns an appetitive ('good') value to the same odour. Ecologically, this makes sense because, being a gregarious locust, it should find and eat toxic plants to defend itself against predators.
"Then we asked, if a solitary locust has already learned about an odour and then it finds itself in a crowd, what would happen to its memories? Can it switch the value that it has assigned to the odour, or more precisely, does crowding change the value of a previous memory from aversive to appetitive?
We found that locusts cannot do this: they are stuck with the value of their already acquired memories. However, strikingly we found that locusts in this transitional period also cannot form any new aversive memories, while they can still form new appetitive memories.
"We then simulated the context in which the switch to swarming behaviour takes place in the field. We show that this simple, selective block of aversion learning enables the locusts to effectively re-train themselves to learn that the same odour that indicated 'bad' now indicates 'good'."
As Dr. Ott explains: "Desert locusts aggregate into swarms when they run out of food — the crowding is driven by hunger and competition for the last few plants in the desert. They are pretty desperate when they transform into the gregarious phase, so they will give the toxic plants another try. And because they can no longer form aversive memories, any food is now rewarding and they form the new 'correct' appetitive memory with the odour. This is how they re-train themselves to eat the toxic plants. It would be great to be able to do this experiment in the field but it would be quite tricky."
Dr Niven adds: "Because newly crowded locusts don't form memories about toxins they ingest, all they remember is the pleasant side of what they ate, and they ignore the toxin. In this way, a smell previously associated with a toxin can become associated with a pleasant experience.
"The changes in learning and memory we're proposing don't require the locusts to understand what's happening to them — they just have to feed and form associations."
The study recalls the work of Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov who famously studied dogs salivating in anticipation of food. Pavlov rang a bell every time he presented the dog with food. After a few 'training sessions' ringing the bell alone was sufficient to make the dog salivate, as it had come to associate the sound of the bell with getting food.
The scientists trained the locusts just like Pavlov trained his dog, except that the unfamiliar smell replaces the bell in Pavlov's experiments: they gently blew vanilla or lemon odour at the restrained locust, while they spoon-fed it with artificial food.
After training, the researchers 'asked' the locust: do you prefer vanilla odour or lemon odour. "We walk the locust on a rod that bifurcates into two arms: one arm has lemon odour, the other vanilla. So the locust has to make up its mind: do I go to vanilla or to lemon. Without training, locusts like vanilla more than lemon. But if you pair nutritious food with lemon during the training, they will go to lemon. And if you train solitary locusts with toxic food and vanilla, they will also go to lemon."
The new results show how brains do not solve problems 'in a vacuum' but in interaction with the environment.
"Simply crowding a locust won't change its mind about the odour being 'bad'," the authors say: "We first thought, this is pretty daft, the locust should now like the odour. But even we humans struggle to forget food poisoning although we know the next meal is fine. The locusts do not operate in a vacuum, they live in the desert. And in the circumstances that they need to re-evaluate the 'meaning' of an odour, it takes only a simple modification of the rules by which they learn: turn off learning 'bad' but keep learning 'good', and the locust can retrain itself.
"So a little brain with simple rules goes a long way if you let it interact with the environment."
INFORMATION:
The company you keep shapes what you learn
Research from Universities of Leicester, Sussex and Cambridge into desert locusts discovers how your environment shapes your thinking
2013-11-21
ELSE PRESS RELEASES FROM THIS DATE:
Growth more stunted in lower-income youth with kidney disease
2013-11-21
Growth more stunted in lower-income youth with kidney disease
NIH-funded study suggests increasing treatment adherence may help
Even with more prescriptions for growth hormone, children and adolescents with chronic ...
Targets of anticancer drugs have broader functions than what their name suggests
2013-11-21
Targets of anticancer drugs have broader functions than what their name suggests
Findings have implications for oncology, diabetes drug development
PHILADELPHIA - Drugs that inhibit the activity of enzymes called histone deacetylases ...
2 human proteins found to affect how 'jumping gene' gets around
2013-11-21
2 human proteins found to affect how 'jumping gene' gets around
Using a new method to catch elusive "jumping genes" in the act, researchers have found two human proteins that are used by one type of DNA to replicate itself and move from place to place. The discovery, ...
Follow the genes: Yale team finds clues to origin of autism
2013-11-21
Follow the genes: Yale team finds clues to origin of autism
Finding major new clues to the origins of autism, a Yale-led team of researchers has pinpointed which cell types and regions of the developing human brain are affected by gene mutations linked to autism. ...
UT Dallas study: Initial success for new tinnitus treatment
2013-11-21
UT Dallas study: Initial success for new tinnitus treatment
UT Dallas researchers have demonstrated that treating tinnitus, or ringing in the ears, using vagus nerve stimulation-tone therapy is safe and brought significant improvement to some of the participants ...
Intestinal bacteria influence food transit through the gut
2013-11-21
Intestinal bacteria influence food transit through the gut
Food transit through the small intestine affects the body's absorption of nutrients and, consequently, our health. The discovery that food transit time is regulated by a hormone indicates new ways to ...
Attractants prevent nerve cell migration
2013-11-21
Attractants prevent nerve cell migration
Researchers from Bonn University decode a roadblock to nerve cell transplantation
A vision is to implant nerve precursor cells in the diseased brains of patients with Parkinson's and Huntington's diseases, whereby ...
Playing computer games makes brains feel and think alike
2013-11-21
Playing computer games makes brains feel and think alike
It's well known that people who communicate face-to-face will start to imitate each other. People adopt each other's poses and gestures, much like infectious yawning. What is less ...
New publication studies urban environments of Manchester from a qualitative perspective
2013-11-21
New publication studies urban environments of Manchester from a qualitative perspective
Dr Alexander Bridger, Senior Lecturer in Psychology at the University of Huddersfield, has published a new paper that explores ways to qualitatively study the urban environments ...
Captive breeding for thousands of years has impaired olfactory functions in silkmoths
2013-11-21
Captive breeding for thousands of years has impaired olfactory functions in silkmoths
Domesticated silkmoths Bombyx mori have a much more limited perception of environmental odors compared to their wild relatives; The extremely sensitive olfactory ...
LAST 30 PRESS RELEASES:
Hormone therapy reshapes the skeleton in transgender individuals who previously blocked puberty
Evaluating performance and agreement of coronary heart disease polygenic risk scores
Heart failure in zero gravity— external constraint and cardiac hemodynamics
Amid record year for dengue infections, new study finds climate change responsible for 19% of today’s rising dengue burden
New study finds air pollution increases inflammation primarily in patients with heart disease
AI finds undiagnosed liver disease in early stages
The American Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene and the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation announce new research fellowship in malaria genomics in honor of professor Dominic Kwiatkowski
Excessive screen time linked to early puberty and accelerated bone growth
First nationwide study discovers link between delayed puberty in boys and increased hospital visits
Traditional Mayan practices have long promoted unique levels of family harmony. But what effect is globalization having?
New microfluidic device reveals how the shape of a tumour can predict a cancer’s aggressiveness
Speech Accessibility Project partners with The Matthew Foundation, Massachusetts Down Syndrome Congress
Mass General Brigham researchers find too much sitting hurts the heart
New study shows how salmonella tricks gut defenses to cause infection
Study challenges assumptions about how tuberculosis bacteria grow
NASA Goddard Lidar team receives Center Innovation Award for Advancements
Can AI improve plant-based meats?
How microbes create the most toxic form of mercury
‘Walk this Way’: FSU researchers’ model explains how ants create trails to multiple food sources
A new CNIC study describes a mechanism whereby cells respond to mechanical signals from their surroundings
Study uncovers earliest evidence of humans using fire to shape the landscape of Tasmania
Researchers uncover Achilles heel of antibiotic-resistant bacteria
Scientists uncover earliest evidence of fire use to manage Tasmanian landscape
Interpreting population mean treatment effects in the Kansas City Cardiomyopathy Questionnaire
Targeting carbohydrate metabolism in colorectal cancer: Synergy of therapies
Stress makes mice’s memories less specific
Research finds no significant negative impact of repealing a Depression-era law allowing companies to pay workers with disabilities below minimum wage
Resilience index needed to keep us within planet’s ‘safe operating space’
How stress is fundamentally changing our memories
Time in nature benefits children with mental health difficulties: study
[Press-News.org] The company you keep shapes what you learnResearch from Universities of Leicester, Sussex and Cambridge into desert locusts discovers how your environment shapes your thinking