PRESS-NEWS.org - Press Release Distribution
PRESS RELEASES DISTRIBUTION

Why do children use loopholes? New research explains the development of intentional misunderstandings in children

2025-03-12
(Press-News.org)

Most people are familiar with loopholes. When your boss, landlord, partner, customer, or government asks you to do something you don’t want to do, and yet you can’t say “no,” you may resort to malicious compliance – doing what someone asked, but not actually what they meant. Most parents are probably familiar with such “little lawyer” behavior too: if a parent says, “Time to put the tablet down,” a child might physically put the tablet down on the table – and then keep playing on it. While such intentional misunderstandings are common and important, there has been little research on the development of loophole behavior across childhood. 

In a new Child Development study from the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD), researchers at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology and Harvard University examined when and why children engage in loopholes. Tracing the origin of loophole behavior and how communication goes wrong can help us to better understand how communication often goes right, when people are motivated to cooperate. 

To understand more about how, when, and why children use loopholes, 260 parents were asked to report on their children’s (N = 425; 3-18 years old; 42% female; 34% White) use of loopholes. The study team paired this with actual children’s (N = 298; 4-10 years old; 49% female; majority White) behavior in experiments across three studies. 

The research showed that over time, children learn to distinguish loopholes from compliance (cooperation) and non-compliance (refusal). Children from four to ten years of age expect that loopholes will help decrease the amount of trouble they would get into, and they are more likely to use them when they don’t want to do what their parent asks. In particular, the researchers found that loophole behavior typically starts around five to six years of age and then peaks in frequency around seven to eight years. In addition to standard questions, the researchers also examined whether children thought loopholes were funny, looking at their smiles and laughter. Consistent with children’s behavior and with parent’s report, children thought loopholes were amusing, while other behaviors were not. This may be the first research to explore the phenomenon of loopholes across development and can help contribute to a broader understanding of social reasoning. 

SRCD had the opportunity to speak about this research with Kiera Parece, a co-author of the paper and graduate student in the Early Childhood Cognition Lab at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. 

SRCD: Can you please provide a brief overview of the study?

Ms. Kiera Parece: Our work examined children’s use and understanding of “loopholes.” Loopholes are intentional misunderstandings, when a request is technically satisfied but the underlying spirit is violated. They’re common in law, history, and everyday life, but as far as we know no one has examined how and when they develop in childhood. We combined a naturalistic parent survey with three empirical studies in which we asked children to evaluate loopholes, predict when someone will use loopholes, and come up with loopholes of their own. We found that over time children start to distinguish “loopholes” from both compliance and non-compliance. Children expected loopholes to result in less trouble compared to outright defiance and predicted that other children will use loopholes more when there’s a conflict between the goals of a parent and child. While not all children use loopholes, and not all of them develop their use at the same time, the developmental picture that emerged was strongly robust across the studies and parental report: Children’s understanding and use of loopholes seems to emerge around ages five to six and then peak around ages seven to eight.

Children also thought loopholes were funny: They smiled and laughed more when they heard stories about loopholes than when hearing stories about children doing what they were told or outright refusing to do so. While younger children struggled to come up with ways of being sneaky, from five to seven years of age children were increasingly able to come up with valid loopholes on their own. 

SRCD: What led you to study loophole behavior in children? 

Ms. Kiera Parece: First of all, loopholes are just super interesting. And once you start to think about them, they’re everywhere: law, history, fables, rebellions, everyday life, governments, corporations, you name it. And they’re also important: loopholes are something in between complying and not complying, they can get you out of trouble, they’re creative. They’re also worrying: we as humans are currently developing new forms of machine intelligence, which may not do what we want. If we end up building a genie, we better understand genies! 

With all that in mind – that loopholes are important, and pervasive, and worrying– it makes sense to think about where they come from. If we understand when and why children start using loopholes, we’ll have a better understanding of this phenomenon, and of communication more generally. Also, it probably helped that the senior author’s child (Dr. Ullman’s son) was around five or six when we got interested in this, so there was constant pressure at home to better understand this behavior.  

SRCD: Did you learn anything that surprised you? 

Ms. Kiera Parece: One thing that delighted and surprised me was the creativity and wittiness of children’s loopholes, which can be seen in the parent anecdotes we collected, and also the more official experiments we ran. We saw that children engage in loopholes across a wide range of domains. It’s not just “oh, they learned what the word “can” means, so now they can use a loophole that involves ‘can.’” Everything from technology (a child is told no computer, so they use a tablet) to eating sweets (a child is told no more gummy bears, they switch to gummy worms), to spatial reasoning, to the understanding of time and relationships, children were finding creative, diverse ways around all of this. 

The other thing that was non-obvious to me going into this is the specific robust pattern that we found: Children generally start to use and understand loopholes around ages five to six, it then peaks around seven to eight and falls off from there. It’s interesting that it doesn’t robustly start before then, and to think: why not? Children already understand a lot of pragmatics, pretense, theory of mind, and other ingredients of loopholes at younger ages, so why this pattern? I’m sure parents of five to six-year-olds will not be surprised to hear other kids do this, but I hope it gets them to think about why children do this, and why at this age.

SRCD: Can you please explain how this research might be helpful for researchers, parents, caregivers, teachers and others who work with children? 

Ms. Kiera Parece: Absolutely. Parents, caregivers, teachers, or researchers who work with kids are often facing this basic dynamic: they want to get a child to do something. They have some kind of goal for the kid (whether a chore, or something the child needs to learn, or whatever), and they need to communicate that goal to the child. But the child has their own goals, and their goal might not be the same as the parent or teacher or caregiver.

Usually, this process is so obvious and transparent we don’t even think about it: we tell the child what we want, and the child either does it or does not do it. The use of loopholes throws that process off balance. It’s like a visual illusion; it’s a breakdown that gets you to think about a process that usually works fine. In this case, it highlights that all language is fuzzy, and that cooperative communication involves a process in which the child first needs to understand what we want and then decide what to do about that. So, we hope this research highlights for teachers and caregivers that everyday communication is actually pretty special, that loopholes are not just something one child does but something a lot of kids do, that it can be creative and witty and funny, and that it is the cornerstone for a behavior we see all over the place as adults. 

More generally, we also think this research is helpful for policymakers who are in a similar position to parents. That is, instead of a parent trying to get a child to do something, think of an authority figure more generally trying to get someone within their authority to do something, whether it is the government, the law, or engineers developing artificial intelligence systems. This research is helpful in understanding how intentional misunderstandings emerge, which hopefully sets the foundation for how to avoid them in cases where we want to avoid them, or how to use them in cases where we want to encourage them. 

SRCD: Can you please address some of the research limitations? 

Ms. Kiera Parece: Great point, and like any research this one had limitations too. The children who participated in our studies were based in the United States, and it’s possible that differences in culture and parenting styles play a role in the emergence and use of loopholes. Also, we focused on children’s relationship with parents, but they likely use loopholes in other situations and relationships as well, ones that vary in intimacy (say, teachers and babysitters) or hierarchy (think of the difference between a parent asking you to do something or a younger sibling asking you to do something. You may use a loophole with your parents, but you might just tell your younger brother ‘no!’). 

In addition, we focused on children from four to ten years of age. While we found a very interesting transition there, it leaves out parts of the developmental picture. Additional work with teenagers and toddlers can tell a clearer story of the development of loopholes. 

SRCD: What’s next in this field of research?

Ms. Kiera Parece: There’s so many interesting questions still open when it comes to loopholes in general, and loopholes in children more specifically. 

To pick a few of those: 

First, while we found a robust and non-obvious developmental pattern in understanding loopholes, more work is needed to understand *why* the transition is happening at those ages. Why don’t children start using loopholes robustly around ages three to four, or eight to nine? It probably has something to do with the specific cognitive skills necessary to engage with them, so that charts a clear research direction that looks more directly at the correlation between the use of loopholes and other developmental phenomena. 

On top of that, we found that children believe that loopholes will lead to less trouble than simply defying a request. But it doesn’t explain why they think that. We’re not short on answers there, it’s just that there are several and it isn’t clear which is the right one. For example, maybe loopholes get you out of trouble because they are funny and creative. Or maybe it is because they provide ‘plausible deniability’, so a child can claim they generally didn’t understand the request. 

But that just raises more questions. For example, why are loopholes funny? They very much are, that’s just an empirical fact, and kids thought they were funny too. But it’s not obvious why acting on the letter of the law as opposed to the spirit of the law makes even a five-year-old crack up. We have some thoughts on this, but it’ll take more research to figure out. 

And for “plausible deniability”, the more you think about it the weirder that explanation sounds. The child who was told to ‘put the tablet down’ and put it on the table only to keep playing games on it, can they honestly claim they were confused? The parent knows what they meant, the child knows what they meant, the child knows the parent knows what they meant, what is ‘plausible’ about a loophole? Finally, I’m sure parents, teachers, caregivers, and AI engineers would appreciate knowing what can make loopholes more or less pervasive. 

This work was supported by the National Science Foundation’s (NSF) Science of Learning and Augmented Intelligence program and the Jacobs Foundation.

Summarized from an article in Child Development, “Learning Loopholes: The Development of Intentional Misunderstandings in Children” by Bridgers, S., Parece, K., Iwasaki I., Broski, A., Schulz, L., (Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology) and Ullman, T., (Department of Psychology at Harvard University). Copyright 2025 The Society for Research in Child Development. All rights reserved. 

END



ELSE PRESS RELEASES FROM THIS DATE:

How satisfied are you with your mattress? New research survey aims to find out

2025-03-12
Sleep quality is a crucial aspect of health, yet while adults spend around a third of their lives sleeping, there is surprisingly little research on mattresses. Mass General Brigham researchers developed and tested the Boston Mattress Satisfaction Questionnaire (BMSQ), a new tool to rigorously assess mattress satisfaction and characteristics. They administered the BMSQ to a representative sample of over 1,000 adults in the United States, finding that the tool is internally consistent and viable for assessing mattress ...

Democracy first? Economic model begs to differ

Democracy first? Economic model begs to differ
2025-03-12
Recent studies on economic growth report that preventing the abuse of state power through democratic institutions is critical to a nation’s development. However, there has been little prior research on how societies transition in response to the two conflicting goals of limiting the state’s stranglehold on governance while improving its administrative capacity through citizens’ political participation. Osaka Metropolitan University Associate Professor Ryosuke Okazawa of the Graduate School of Economics led a team that ...

Opening a new chapter in 3D microprinting with the dream material 'MXene'!

Opening a new chapter in 3D microprinting with the dream material MXene!
2025-03-12
The Smart 3D Printing Research Team at KERI, led by Dr. Seol Seung-kwon, has developed the world’s first technology for printing high-resolution 3D microstructures using 'MXene,' a material known as the dream material. MXene, first discovered in the United States in 2011, is a two-dimensional nanomaterial composed of alternating metal and carbon layers. MXene possesses high electrical conductivity and electromagnetic shielding capabilities. Due to its easy combination with various metal chemicals, MXene has gained significant attention ...

Temperature during development influences connectivity between neurons and behavior in fruit flies

Temperature during development influences connectivity between neurons and behavior in fruit flies
2025-03-12
The rate of development of poikilothermic animals, such as insects, fish, and reptiles, is determined by environmental temperature. A research team at Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz (JGU) has recently demonstrated how temperature can affect brain development in fruit flies. "In the area of the brain we examined, neurons formed more synapses and connected to more synaptic partners at lower temperatures," stated Dr. Carlotta Martelli, head of the team at the Institute of Developmental Biology and Neurobiology of JGU. In their study, the scientists focused on the olfactory circuit of Drosophila melanogaster, because the sense of smell determines important behavioral patterns in ...

Are you just tired or are you menopause tired?

2025-03-12
CLEVELAND, Ohio (March 12, 2025)—Multiple menopause symptoms can make women feel fatigued. Hot flashes, sleep problems, pain, and depression are just a few. A new study suggests that heavy or prolonged menstrual bleeding may increase fatigue, which helps to explain why midlife women are two-to-four times more likely to experience debilitating forms of syndromic fatigue. Results of the study are published online today in Menopause, the journal of The Menopause Society. Most women transitioning through ...

Fluorescent dope

Fluorescent dope
2025-03-12
Kyoto, Japan -- We're all familiar with Pavlovian conditioning, in which a reward-anticipatory behavior follows a reward-predicting stimulus. Perhaps you experience it yourself when passing a café or restaurant and catching a whiff of something delectable. Behind this mechanism is dopamine released within the striatum, the largest structure of the subcortical basal ganglia, which links motor movements and motivation. Yet it has remained unclear exactly what kind of dopamine signal is transmitted to the striatum to cause this behavior in primates. In order to understand this dopamine signal, a team of researchers from Kyoto University ...

Meningococcal vaccine found to be safe and effective for infants in sub-Saharan Africa

2025-03-12
University of Maryland School of Medicine (UMSOM) researchers helped conduct an important new global health study that found a vaccine that protects against five strains of meningitis prevalent in sub-Saharan Africa is safe and effective for use in young children beginning at 9 months of age. This study provided evidence that formed the basis for the World Health Organization’s (WHO) decision last year to recommend the pentavalent Men5CV meningitis vaccine for infants ages 9 months and older. Results of the study were published today in the journal Lancet. In January 2024, the WHO recommended that all countries in the African meningitis belt introduce Men5CV into their routine immunization ...

Integrating stopping smoking support into talking therapies helps more people quit – new study

2025-03-12
Research from the University of Bath, funded by Cancer Research UK, published on No Smoking Day 2025, shows that integrating smoking cessation support into NHS Talking Therapies for depression and anxiety increases quit rates. This is an important step in addressing the high rates of smoking in this population. Led by Dr Gemma Taylor at the University of Bath, alongside researchers from several other institutions, the study published in Addiction also found that adding smoking support to mental health treatment didn’t disrupt therapy. Instead, it offered a practical way to tackle mental and physical health together. About the Study The trial was conducted across four NHS ...

Breast cancer death rates will rise in elderly EU patients but fall for all other ages

2025-03-12
Death rates from breast cancer will fall in 2025 in every age group in the EU and the UK apart from in EU patients aged 80 years and older. In these older patients, overall mortality rates from the disease are predicted to rise by nearly 10% in 2025. However, in the UK, breast cancer death rates are expected to decrease by 7% in this age group, compared to rates observed between 2015-2019. In Spain, there is also a 4% decrease in patients aged 80 and over. These findings are from a new study published in the leading cancer journal Annals of Oncology [1] today (Wednesday), ...

Routine asthma test more reliable in the morning and has seasonal effects, say doctors

2025-03-12
A lung function test used to help diagnose asthma works better in the morning, becoming less reliable throughout the day, Cambridge researchers have found. Using real world data from 1,600 patients, available through a database created for speeding up research and innovation, the team also found that its reliability differs significantly in winter compared to autumn. Asthma is a common lung condition that can cause wheezing and shortness of breath, occasionally severe. Around 6.5% of people over six years old in the UK are affected by the condition. Treatments include the use of inhalers or nebulisers to carry ...

LAST 30 PRESS RELEASES:

Thirty-three centers join new Bronchiectasis and NTM Care Center Network

Effects of ethanol on the digestive system

KIER unveils blueprint for cost-effective production of eco-friendly green hydrogen

Blind to the burn: Misconceptions about skin cancer risk in the US

Young Australians demand action on mental health, cost of living and education reform: report

First national perception survey of Food is Medicine programs shows strong public support

UNCG professor investigates how symbiotic groups can behave like single organisms with $600,000 in Templeton Foundation funding

Targeted alpha therapy: a breakthrough in treating refractory skin cancer

Transforming thymic carcinoma treatment with a dual approach

Wrong on skin cares: keratinocytes, not fibroblasts, make collagen for healthy skin

Delhi air pollution worse than expected as water vapour skews figures

First radio pulses traced to dead-star binary

New membrane discovery makes possible cleaner lithium extraction

Entwined dwarf stars reveal their location thanks to repeated radio bursts

Landscape scale pesticide pollution detected in the Upper Rhine region, from agricultural lowlands to remote areas

Decoding nanomaterial phase transitions with tiny drums

Two-star system explains unusual astrophysical phenomenon

Minimal TV viewing may be protective for heart diseases linked to Type 2 diabetes

Mass General Brigham study finds relationship between doomsday clock and patterns of mortality and mental health in the united states

Signs of ‘tipping point’ to electric vehicles in UK used car market

A new name for one of the world's rarest rhinoceroses

Why do children use loopholes? New research explains the development of intentional misunderstandings in children

How satisfied are you with your mattress? New research survey aims to find out

Democracy first? Economic model begs to differ

Opening a new chapter in 3D microprinting with the dream material 'MXene'!

Temperature during development influences connectivity between neurons and behavior in fruit flies

Are you just tired or are you menopause tired?

Fluorescent dope

Meningococcal vaccine found to be safe and effective for infants in sub-Saharan Africa

Integrating stopping smoking support into talking therapies helps more people quit – new study

[Press-News.org] Why do children use loopholes? New research explains the development of intentional misunderstandings in children