PRESS-NEWS.org - Press Release Distribution
PRESS RELEASES DISTRIBUTION

Will neuroscience challenge the legal concept of criminal responsibility?

2011-06-03
(Press-News.org) Just before 10:00 a.m. on June 20, 2001, a uniformed police officer was dispatched to do what he thought was a routine welfare check at a home in Houston, Texas. When the officer met Andrea Yates at the door, she immediately told him, "I just killed my kids." When Yates was later asked why she drowned her five children, she claimed she had to in order to save them from hell. The police would learn that Yates had been suffering from long-term post-partum depression and psychosis.

Nearly 10 years after Andrea Yates killed her five children, the case remains hotly debated because of the critical legal questions it and other cases involving criminal insanity raise. At her first trial, Yates was convicted of capital murder. After her successful appeal, however, a second jury found that Yates' post-partum psychosis rendered her incapable of distinguishing between right and wrong when she murdered her children. She was then civilly committed to a high-security treatment facility—the usual consequence of a successful insanity-based acquittal.(1)

What role did Yates' severe post-partum psychosis play in the crime? Was she criminally responsible for the murders of her children given her mental illness? And, more broadly, how can we as a society successfully balance protecting our citizens from crimes while still protecting those with debilitating mental disorders? These are questions that go far beyond this one crime—and that many argue will be strongly influenced by future neuroscientific studies.

Personal Responsibility and the Law

Most Americans are familiar with one of the most basic concepts of our judicial system—the idea that every person is innocent until proven guilty. A finding of guilt requires that the prosecution prove the offense beyond a reasonable doubt. But even if that criterion is met, the defendant can still avoid conviction by establishing an affirmative defense of justification or excuse. In the former case, conduct that would otherwise be considered criminal is permissible under specific circumstances, like in cases of self-defense. In cases of excuse, the defendant may have acted criminally but is not considered a responsible agent because of infancy or legal insanity. If one examines the excusing conditions, it is clear that the capacity for rationality and, more controversially, the capacity for control are necessary for legal guilt. The important assumption underlying the ideal of only punishing blameworthy agents is that each person is a rational individual, able to differentiate right from wrong and take responsibility for his or her own actions. Demonstrating lack of rational capacity can be difficult, even in cases involving those suffering from an obvious and severe mental illness like Andrea Yates.

Consider murder, says Stephen J. Morse, a professor of law and professor of psychology and law in psychiatry at the University of Pennsylvania. The criteria for murder are an intentional killing act done with the purpose of killing the victim. For example, if the prosecution can prove beyond a reasonable doubt that an individual shot a person with the express intent of killing him or her, then the killer will be prima facie guilty of murder. If the individual is severely mentally ill, for example, and met the other requirements for legal insanity, such as not knowing right from wrong at the time of the crime, then he or she may be considered not guilty by reason of mental disease or defect.(2)

The M'Naghten Rules

In 1843, a Scottish laborer named Daniel M'Naghten tried to assassinate the British Prime Minister, Robert Peel, while in the throes of a paranoid delusion and instead killed Peel's secretary, Edward Drummond. This one act inspired Great Britain's House of Lords to establish the so-called M'Naghten Rules, guidelines for dealing with criminal defendants who are mentally incompetent in some manner. The questions at the heart of these rules are whether defendants know what they were doing when they committed the crime and if they knew it was wrong. If the answer to one or both is no, then a plea of not guilty by reason of insanity (or in more modern legal language, by reason of mental disease or defect) may be accepted by the judge or jury. But it is a difficult thing to prove.

"The rules have basically remained unchanged since the mid-nineteenth century," says the Honorable Matthew J. D'Emic, a judge who presides over the Brooklyn Mental Health Court in New York State. "And I think the reason for that is that it still holds up." The M'Naghten Rules remain the dominant test of rationality in the United States, but may be supplemented with control tests and other rules in some jurisdictions.

Though popular crime dramas on television suggest that insanity defenses are a dime a dozen, in reality, they are quite rare. "An insanity defense has to be proven by the defendant by a preponderance of the evidence," says D'Emic. "And it's a very difficult burden." In some jurisdictions, defense attorneys must demonstrate that defendants' mental conditions, at the moment of the crime, rendered them unable to know what they were doing or differentiate right from wrong. During Andrea Yates' first trial, the jury found her guilty of capital murder after hearing she had waited until her husband left for work to commit the crime, kenneled their dog and called the police—behavior which suggests she knew her acts were wrong.

"It was a very difficult case because everything turned on how broadly or narrowly the applicable insanity criteria were interpreted," says Morse. "Did Andrea Yates kill her children intentionally? Certainly. Did she know what she was doing? In a narrow sense, yes. But in the broader sense, she did not understand because her fundamental material reason for action, saving them from hell, was deluded."

Can Neuroscience Lessen the Burden?

Some hope that advances in neuroscience can help relieve some of the burden on defense attorneys representing mentally ill clients. For example, researchers are currently using neuroimaging and genetic techniques to better understand both the causes and progression of conditions like psychosis and psychopathy. In a fiercely debated case like that of Andrea Yates, it is hoped that a particular result on a genetic test or specific anomalies observed on a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scan may one day help jurors better discern whether a defendant suffered from a particular condition—and perhaps even whether the defendant should be considered responsible for his or her actions.

The problem, however, is that neuroscience is not there yet. Though a brain scan may be able to one day support a diagnosis of a particular condition—the science is still too immature to do at present—it cannot offer evidence of a particular mental state.(3)(4)

"Neuroscientists say they are capturing mental states with various types of brain-imaging techniques," says Michael S. Gazzaniga, director, Sage Center for the Study of Mind at University of California, Santa Barbara. "We say we are learning to understand the neural mechanisms of a person intending to do something. But is that really true? Can you really recreate, at one point in time, what someone's intention might have been? We can't—at least not at this time."

But that's not to say that brain scans and other neuroscientific evidence will not be deemed permissible in future court rooms. Walter Sinnott-Armstrong, a neurophilosopher at Duke University's Kenan Institute for Ethics maintains that defendants should be able to—and likely soon will be able to—use scans demonstrating brain lesions and tumors in certain cases, even though they are only statistically associated with legally relevant cognitive disabilities.

"Courts allow boot prints, handwriting analyses and all kinds of other types of evidence that are a long way from proof. And neuroscience can work together with other sources to provide evidence of crucial mental states and conditions," he says. Yet, Martha J. Farah, director of the Center for Neuroscience and Society at the University of Pennsylvania, questions whether neuroimaging will ever be able to offer us anything more telling than the psychological assessments used today when it comes to intention.

"As it stands, legal questions of responsibility are based on folk psychological concepts of understanding, motivation, reasons, and incentives," she says. "The neuroscience won't supplant any of those folk psychological concepts. It's more likely that the neuroscience will eventually assist doctors and lawyers in determining how to apply these folk psychological concepts to a person by supplying evidence about their cognitive abilities and motivation states." It's often said that brains don't commit crimes—people do.5 And even if a brain scan can confirm a particular condition, it is unlikely that it would be able to give a strict yes or no answer regarding whether a defendant was responsible for an act.

"A major mistake that people make over and over again when thinking about criminal responsibility and neuroscience is that causation is an excusing condition. It's not," says Morse. "Neuroscience is just another part of the full causal explanation of human behavior. Just because a defendant has a cause that is not under his control does not mean he is not responsible for his crime. A person can suffer from a severe and persistent mental illness and yet still, under the law, be responsible for his actions because he does not meet the criteria for an excuse."

Moving Forward

Though neuroscientific results are currently considered more prejudicial than probative in the court room, Gazzaniga argues that, with time, the findings will become robust enough to be used in legal proceedings. What's more, he believes we need to start preparing for that day now.

So how can we as a society successfully balance protecting citizens from crimes while still protecting those with debilitating mental disorders? It's a tricky question. Joshua Buckholtz, an assistant professor at Harvard University who studies the neurogenetic architecture of psychopathology, says that, ultimately, it is a question for the law to decide.(6) He cautions, however, that making those decisions—and, as part of it, defining criminal responsibility as it pertains to those with mental disorders—requires an understanding of what neuroscience really has to offer.

"We need to do a much better job of educating people about what neuroscience is, including its methods and limitations," he says. "Judges' chambers are the crucible for this interaction between neuroscience and law. So, to the extent that we have judges who have better neuroscientific education, I think the better off we as a society will be."

Farah agrees. "There is no magic formula except to keep the channels of communication open," she says. "We need to make sure that scientists understand the legal relevance of what they're doing well enough to be able to constructively contribute to discussions about it. Make sure that lawyers understand the science well enough not to have too much naïve faith in it or be too suspicious of it. Working together is going to guide us towards the right integration of science and law."

###

(The Dana Foundation funds a grant to the AAAS to hold seminars for state and federal judges on emerging issues in neuroscience.)

References:
1 Keram EA. The Insanity Defense and Game Theory: Reflections on Texas v. Yates. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law. 2002, 30(4).
2 Morse SJ. Lost in Translation? An Essay on Law and Neuroscience. In Freeman M (Ed.) Law and Neuroscience: Current Legal Issues 2010. Oxford University Press.
3 Gazzaniga MS. Neuroscience in the Courtroom. Scientific American, April 2011.
4 The Law and Neuroscience Project: http://www.lawandneuroscienceproject.org
5 Morse SJ. Brain Overclaim Syndrome and Criminal Responsibility: A Diagnostic Note. Ohio State Journal of Criminal Law. 2006, 397(400).
6 Kiehl KA and Buckholtz JW. Inside of the Mind of a Psychopath. Scientific American Mind, September/October 2010.

END



ELSE PRESS RELEASES FROM THIS DATE:

Combination therapy shows promise for rare, deadly cancer caused by asbestos

2011-06-03
Philadelphia – Pleural mesothelioma patients who undergo lung-sparing surgery in combination with photodynamic therapy (PDT) show superior overall survival than patient treated using the conventional therapy of extrapleural pneumonectomy (EPP) (or en bloc removal of the lung and surrounding tissue) with PDT, indicates new research from the Raymond and Ruth Perelman School of Medicine at the University of Pennsylvania. The research is published in the June 2011 issue of the Annals of Thoracic Surgery. "Unlike patients who receive traditional lung sacrificing surgery for ...

Defective Clip-On Table-Top Baby Chairs

2011-06-03
On May 6, 2011, the U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission (CPSC) issued an "urgent warning" about the "Me Too" Clip-On Table Top Baby Chairs (imported by phil&teds USA Inc.). The CPSC has determined that this baby chair poses a risk of serious injury to children. The CPSC strongly urges all consumers to stop using the chairs immediately. See http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=hC0B_k0nTlw for the CPSC's 2-minute video announcement and warning. The chair consists of a nylon seat with a metal frame that clamps onto a table edge using two metal vise ...

Climate projections don't accurately reflect soil carbon release

2011-06-03
CORVALLIS, Ore. – A new study concludes that models may be predicting releases of atmospheric carbon dioxide that are either too high or too low, depending on the region, because they don't adequately reflect variable temperatures that can affect the amount of carbon released from soil. The study points out that many global models make estimates of greenhouse gas emissions from soils based on "average" projected temperatures. But temperatures vary widely from those averages. That variability, along with complex biological processes, makes the issue far more complicated. Researchers ...

Is the description-experience gap in risky choice limited to rare events?

2011-06-03
Psychology researchers at the University of Alberta have found an interesting wrinkle in the decision- making process people use when gambling: People confronted with risky choices respond differently when they rely on past experiences, rather than when they just focus on the odds of winning or losing. The research team gave people two kinds of choices. One was a choice between a sure win versus a double-or-nothing win. The other choice was between a sure loss versus a double-or- nothing loss. In some cases the odds were explained to the volunteer gamblers, and sometimes ...

Color red increases the speed and strength of reactions

Color red increases the speed and strength of reactions
2011-06-03
What links speed, power, and the color red? Hint: it's not a sports car. It's your muscles. A new study, published in the latest issue of the journal Emotion, finds that when humans see red, their reactions become both faster and more forceful. And people are unaware of the color's intensifying effect. The findings may have applications for sporting and other activities in which a brief burst of strength and speed is needed, such as weightlifting. But the authors caution that the color energy boost is likely short-lived. "Red enhances our physical reactions because ...

Federal Tort Reform Bill Would Cap Medical Malpractice Awards

2011-06-03
Everyone has heard horrific tales of medical negligence -- the healthy body part removed instead of the cancerous one, the adult dose of medication given to a child, the baby starting life with a permanent birth injury because someone made a mistake, the grandparent who dies from a hospital infection. In our legal system, a person hurt through the negligence of another can sue in court for money damages for the harm received. In most scenarios, a jury decides how much money would compensate a victim for his or her injury at the negligent or malicious hand of another. It ...

This is what the margins of the Ebro looked like 6 million years ago

This is what the margins of the Ebro looked like 6 million years ago
2011-06-03
A Spanish research team, using 3D reflection seismology, has for the first time mapped the geomorphological features of the Ebro river basin between five and six million years ago. The images obtained show that the surface analysed is today 2.5 or 3 kilometres below the sea bed. "The results shed light on the way in which the sea level fell during the Messinian (between 5.33 and 6 million years ago), and imply that the subsequent inundation of the river margins happened extremely quickly", Roger Urgeles, lead author of the study and a researcher at the Department of Marine ...

Good News for Bankruptcy in New York

2011-06-03
On his way out of office in December 2010, now former New York Gov. David Patterson signed legislation giving people filing for bankruptcy something to celebrate. The bill raises the limits on several state property exemptions and allows debtors to choose between New York and Federal exemption limits. Property exemptions are the assets protected from creditors trying to satisfy a money judgment during bankruptcy proceedings. New York's exemptions fall into the following categories: - Homestead - Motor vehicle - Cash - Jewelry and art - Tool of trade - Cash, household ...

Noninvasive diagnostics may offer alternative to liver biopsy for assessing liver fibrosis

2011-06-03
Patients who are evaluated for liver diseases such as hepatitis C (HCV) are typically recommended for liver biopsy to determine the extent of disease progression. For patients who question whether less invasive testing is available, clinicians now have alternatives options to consider. Elastography and serum markers are two such diagnostic options reviewed in an editorial published in the June issue of Hepatology, a journal of the American Association for the Study of Liver Diseases. Epidemiological studies report that approximately 150,000 individuals in the U.S. are ...

New bitter blocker discovered

2011-06-03
PHILADELPHIA (June 2, 2011) -- Although bitterness can sometimes be desirable – such as in the taste of coffee or chocolate – more often bitter taste causes rejection that can interfere with food selection, nutrition and therapeutic compliance. This is especially true for children. Now, scientists from the Monell Center and Integral Molecular describe the discovery of a compound that inhibits bitterness by acting directly on a subset of bitter taste receptors. "Bitter taste is a major problem for pediatric drug compliance and also for proper nutrition, such as eating ...

LAST 30 PRESS RELEASES:

Intelligent fight: AI enhances cervical cancer detection

Breakthrough study reveals the secrets behind cordierite’s anomalous thermal expansion

Patient-reported influence of sociopolitical issues on post-Dobbs vasectomy decisions

Radon exposure and gestational diabetes

EMBARGOED UNTIL 1600 GMT, FRIDAY 10 JANUARY 2025: Northumbria space physicist honoured by Royal Astronomical Society

Medicare rules may reduce prescription steering

Red light linked to lowered risk of blood clots

Menarini Group and Insilico Medicine enter a second exclusive global license agreement for an AI discovered preclinical asset targeting high unmet needs in oncology

Climate fee on food could effectively cut greenhouse gas emissions in agriculture while ensuring a social balance

Harnessing microwave flow reaction to convert biomass into useful sugars

Unveiling the secrets of bone strength: the role of biglycan and decorin

Revealing the “true colors” of a single-atom layer of metal alloys

New data on atmosphere from Earth to the edge of space

Self-destructing vaccine offers enhanced protection against tuberculosis in monkeys

Feeding your good gut bacteria through fiber in diet may boost body against infections

Sustainable building components create a good indoor climate

High levels of disordered eating among young people linked to brain differences

Hydrogen peroxide and the mystery of fruit ripening: ‘Signal messengers’ in plants

T cells’ capability to fully prevent acute viral infections opens new avenues for vaccine development

Study suggests that magma composition drives volcanic tremor

Sea surface temperatures and deeper water temperatures reached a new record high in 2024

Connecting through culture: Understanding its relevance in intercultural lingua franca communication

Men more than three times as likely to die from a brain injury, new US study shows

Tongue cancer organoids reveal secrets of chemotherapy resistance

Applications, limitations, and prospects of different muscle atrophy models in sarcopenia and cachexia research

FIFAWC: A dataset with detailed annotation and rich semantics for group activity recognition

Transfer learning-enhanced physics-informed neural network (TLE-PINN): A breakthrough in melt pool prediction for laser melting

Holistic integrative medicine declaration

Hidden transport pathways in graphene confirmed, paving the way for next-generation device innovation

New Neurology® Open Access journal announced

[Press-News.org] Will neuroscience challenge the legal concept of criminal responsibility?